The Winter of Scholarly Science Journals
Irving Maltzman, Ph.D.
Science,
idealized, is the objective, disinterested, communal search for the
truth. One of its important virtues is that it is self-correcting
(Merton, 1968).
Fraud,
purportedly a rare occurrence, is uncovered and punished, and cannot
ordinarily escape exposure because science replicates its results,
especially its important results, and these results are published in
journals following peer review. This traditional view is based
upon a number of unexamined assumptions. We can deal here briefly with
only one of these: the critical role of scholarly journals as
objective, disinterested vehicles for the expression of dissent and
criticism generally and the exposure of fraud in science in particular.
By fraud I mean the intentional misrepresentation of any one or more of
the following: procedures, results, or the conclusions and implications
of research.
Among the important factors that may interfere with the idealized
vision of scholarly science journals are the following:
One:
The selective filter of ideology and politics, which may interfere with
the expression of criticism and the exposure of fraud at many levels.
By ideology I do not mean elaborated economic or political systems that
interfere with the free expression of science, but rather the
masquerading of value statements as statements of fact (Bergmann,
1954). If one ideology in this latter sense dominates an area of study
or research, there are few critical, questioning adversaries serving as
watchdogs; fraud is less likely to be detected under such
circumstances. If detected, it is less likely to be reported by fellow
ideologues and, if reported, less likely to be acted upon. Interference
with the process of self-correction may also occur because adherents to
a common ideology react to an attack upon or criticism of one of their
members as an attack upon themselves. They may therefore attempt to
avoid, defend against, or suppress such an attack. Ideologues are more
forgiving of each other than of their common foe. It may be all
rationalized as serving a greater good. Critical peer reviews and
scholarship suffer in such an atmosphere.
Detection of fraud and its reporting are most likely to occur from an
adversarial quarter, not from friends or fellow ideologues. Diversity
and pluralism, in science as in other institutions and in society at
large, help safeguard against fraud and facilitates its exposure when
it is detected. Deadening effects of ideology on self-correction may be
present to a far greater degree than casual observers imagine.
Two:
The chilling effect of libel and the threat of libel, which is an
obstacle to the reporting of fraud and the expression of criticism.
Idealized visions of science assume that there is free access to
primary scholarly journals and that editorial reviews are conducted in
an objective, disinterested manner. Quality determines probability of
publication, not ideology and legal considerations. Unfortunately, this
has not been my experience in an attempt to expose an alleged case of
fraud in the field of alcoholism treatment, nor has it been my
invariable experience in less controversial areas of academic
biobehavioral science.
Beginning in the early 1970s and culminating in the publication of a
book, Mark and Linda Sobell (1978) have reported and widely circulated
the claim that they successfully treated physically dependent chronic
alcoholics — so-called gamma alcoholics — to return to controlled,
moderate drinking. They reported finding a significantly greater number
of these alcoholics functioning well at the end of a two-year follow-up
period than gamma alcoholics receiving the traditional treatment goal
of abstinence. A third-year independent follow-up by Caddy, Addington,
and Perkins (1978), encouraged and assisted by the Sobells, purportedly
confirmed the Sobells' findings.
Two factors contributed to the Sobells' enormous impact and attention in the scholarly literature and subsequently in textbooks:
1)
They used behavior modification techniques, the application of
principles presumably derived from the laboratory to problems of
maladaptive behavior. Behavior modification (and its variations) was
becoming the major new ideology in clinical psychology and was winning
the battle for allegiance of newer clinical psychologists over the
older, “nonscientific” psychodynamic approaches. There was, therefore,
a large body of receptive clinical psychologists who hailed the
Sobell's work as another successful application of their approach to an
entirely new area of research and treatment.
2)
Their study appeared to be the most rigorous experiment ever conducted
in the field of alcoholism treatment. The study purportedly employed
random assignment of patients to experimental and control groups, and
obtained quantitative data and conducted statistical analyses on daily
drinking dispositions for every day for the full two years of the
follow-up period.
I
became involved in the discovery of alleged fraud committed by the
Sobells, and by Caddy, Addington, and Perkins, entirely by chance. In
1972 a colleague, Mary Pendery, and I visited Patton State Hospital
(San Bernardino, California) where the research had been recently
conducted. Colleagues, including former students of mine on the staff
of the hospital, urged us to follow up the patients. They were
concerned about the validity of the results. Despite its widely
acclaimed success, staff members were seeing rehospitalized patients.
It
was not until some years later that the powerful ideological network
supporting the Sobells became apparent. Through the Freedom of
Information Act, copies of the correspondence between the Sobells and
officials in the recently formed National Institute on Alcohol Abuse
and Alcoholism (NIAAA) were obtained. Thus in September 1973, before we
had initiated our follow-up study, the Sobells wrote to Albert
Powlowski, chief, Extramural Research Branch, expressing concern over
our planned attempt to conduct a follow-up of their patients. They
indicated that they were in consultation with Peter Nathan about
possible courses of action they might take. Nathan was an early
investigator of chronic alcoholics using behavior modification methods
and was considered a senior psychology investigator in the alcoholism
field.
Powlowski
was a participant on a site-visiting team with Nathan as its chair.
They reviewed our proposed research project submitted to NIAAA for
funding to support the conduct of our follow-up beginning in May 1975.
Our grant proposal was rejected as unfeasible. The follow-up was
conducted without extramural funding.
Science
published a report of our follow-up study (Pendery, Maltzman, &
West, 1982) emphasizing only the past and current condition of patients
in the experimental group, showing that the majority were not
functioning well, and at best one was drinking moderately. Science
declined to consider material in an earlier draft of the manuscript
suggesting fraud on the part of the Sobells.
A
copy of this earlier draft was obtained by the Sobells and widely
circulated among their colleagues. G. Alan Marlatt, a major figure in
the behavior therapy/modification approach to alcoholism, wrote on May
27, 1982, to Ray Hodgson, a well-known British psychologist in the
alcoholism field, as follows:
“Their
goal (Pendery and Maltzman) is to destroy the reputation of two
psychologists and to kill the notion of controlled drinking as a viable
treatment option once and for all. And that seems wrong to me.
“I await your response to all this. In the meantime, I am taking the
liberty of sending copies of this letter to Terry Wilson, Peter Nathan
and Barbara McCrady, since I have spoken to each of them recently about
this matter. I am willing to serve as a clearinghouse for all of us who
wish to respond to the issues involved in one way or another. We should
all keep in touch regarding developments in what could become the big
‘gunfight at the O.K. Corral’ in the alcoholism field.”
Marlatt
(1983) authored the leading article in defense of the Sobells and
controlled drinking in the American Psychologist, the journal
distributed to all members of the American Psychological Assn. At that
time Peter Nathan was associate editor of the American Psychologist
responsible for reviewing and accepting articles on clinical issues
including alcoholism. I was not invited to respond to the article.
Mark
and Linda Sobell were invited to provide the standard brief reply to
our report in Science. Instead they replied in a lengthy article in
Behavior Research and Therapy (Sobell & Sobell, 1984a), the leading
journal in the behavior therapy field. I was invited to reply, which I
did (Maltzman, unpublished a), but the editor informed me that my
manuscript was libelous, and it was rejected. Prior to submission, it
had been reviewed and judged to be free of potentially libelous
material by an attorney for the University of California. I then asked
the attorney for Pergamon Press, publisher of Behavior Research and
Therapy, to specify which passages were libelous and the legal
precedents forming the basis for his judgment. He replied that his
judgment stands.
In
August 1984 I wrote a reply (Maltzman, unpublished b) to a comment by
the Sobells (1984b) and a letter to the editor (Maltzman, unpublished
c) in response to a misleading editorial by Marlatt (1984) published in
the Bulletin of the Society of Psychologists in Addictive Behaviors.
The editor declined to publish my comment and letter, stating they were
libelous. I asked him to explain the grounds for such a judgment. He
replied that it was the judgment of the executive committee, which
included Marlatt — who was president of the society at the time. He
also stated that they had decided not to publish further material on
the Sobell controversy and controlled drinking since it was settled. A
lengthy article on controlled drinking appeared in the next issue
(Marlatt, et al., 1985). Similar articles favorable to some form of
controlled drinking and the Sobells' work have continued to appear in
the Bulletin (e.g., Graber & Miller, 1988; Peele, 1986).
In
November 1985 I submitted a manuscript to the British Journal of
Addiction (Maltzman, unpublished d) replying to an article by Doob
(1984). He was the psychologist member of the Dickens Committee
appointed by the president of the Toronto Addiction Research
Foundation, then the employer of the Sobells, to investigate my
allegation that the Sobells had committed fraud. My paper examined some
of the inadequacies that I believed were evident in the report by the
Dickens Committee and the Trachtenberg Committee (Trachtenberg, 1984).
The editor of the Journal wrote that my paper ought to be published but
that in all fairness he wished to obtain replies from all parties
concerned, including the Sobells, Dickens & Doob, and Trachtenberg.
The latter was chair of an investigating committee convened by the
Alcoholism, Drug, and Mental Health Administration (ADAMHA). I
suggested that if he did, the Sobells and others would threaten a libel
suit and inquired what he would do then. He replied that he would cross
that bridge when he came to it. Three years later he came to it — and
rejected my manuscript because of the threat of legal action on the
part of the Sobells, Dickens, and Doob.
My
fourth experience of failure of nerve or influence of ideology on the
editorial policy of a scholarly journal involved a manuscript I sent to
the most prestigious journal in the field, the Journal of Studies on
Alcohol, in January 1986. It was a reply to Cook's article on the
“controlled drinking controversy.”
My
cover letter accompanying the manuscript requested that it not be
reviewed by the field editor for psychology, Marlatt, nor by Nathan,
who at the time was executive editor of the Journal, head of the
Rutgers Center of Alcohol Studies, and president of the corporation
that was publisher of the Journal. My request was honored. It was
reviewed by the editor for the field of sociology and two ad hoc
reviewers, and was accepted with a request for a minor revision, which
was done. After an unduly long delay in its publication, the managing
editor called to say that Nathan found that my manuscript may be
libelous. I wrote to Nathan explaining that the manuscript had been
reviewed by counsel for the University of California and found free of
libelous material. I asked him to indicate which material was libelous.
He never responded to the query.
I
eventually received a letter from the editor stating that they had
rejected my manuscript because an attorney for Rutgers University
judged there was the possibility of a lawsuit against the Journal since
the Sobells stated they might sue the Journal for libel if my article
was published. I replied that I considered this a breach of contract,
among other things.
I
was fortunate enough to obtain the services of a large and prestigious
Los Angeles law firm, which accepted my case pro bono, in public
interest, and brought a complaint against the Journal and others for
breach of contract. This legal action followed repeated attempts by my
attorney to amicably resolve possible differences. His request for
information concerning which specific passages in my paper were
libelous was never honored by the attorney for Rutgers. At considerable
expense to Rutgers University and the taxpayers of New Jersey, the
Journal also hired a large Los Angeles law firm to represent them.
After a delay of more than three and a half years, my paper was
published in the September 1989 issue of the Journal (Maltzman, 1989),
preceded by a Prefatory Comment by Nathan (1989) and followed by
replies from the Sobells (1989), Cook (1989), and Baker (1989).
Nathan's Prefatory Comment is falsified by the objective record,
including legal documents on file in the Superior Court of the County
of Los Angeles (Maltzman v Alcohol Research Documentation, (1988).
Some
good may, nevertheless, come of all this. First, Timothy Baker proved
one of my allegations: The Sobells intentionally misrepresented their
procedures. I have argued that it is logically impossible to report
drinking dispositions for every day of the year derived from interviews
every 3-4 weeks, as did the Sobells, and not know that they were
interviewing subjects less frequently than every 3-4 weeks. I believe
this incorrect reporting of procedures could not be due to
carelessness, as the Dickens Committee Report and the Sobells now
insist.
Baker
(1989) states, “We knew the previous contact date so that we knew
exactly how many days had to be accounted for” (p. 482). The Sobells
therefore knew that, for example, for Subject #14 contacted on March
23, 1972, the previous contact was on Oct. 9, 1971. They knew that
there was a gap of 5.5 months between interviews. They therefore
necessarily knew that subjects were interviewed less frequently than
every 3-4 weeks as reported in their publications. Second, and more
importantly, perhaps, these events demonstrate that complex forces
operate to profoundly intrude upon the processes traditionally assumed
to make for self-correction in science. Much needs to be done to
address problems of fraud in science. These problems are both more
subtle and more complex than they generally are taken to be. The health
and well-being of members of our society as well as science depend upon
corrective action.
# # #
Maltzman's References
Baker, T. An open letter to Journal readers, J. of Studies on Alcohol, Vol. 50, 1989, pp. 481- 483.
Bergmann, G. The Metaphysics of Logical Positivism, Longman,
Green,
New York, 1954. Caddy, G.R.; Addingrton, H.J.,Jr.; and Perkins, D.
Individualized behavior therapy for alcoholics: A third year
independent double-blind follow-up, Behaviour Research & Therapy,
Vol. 16, 1978, pp. 345-362.
Cook,
D.R. Craftsman versus professional: Analysis of the controlled drinking
controversy, J. of studies on Alcohol, Vol. 46, 1985, pp. 433-442.
Cook, D.R. A reply to Maltzman, J. of Studies on Alcohol, Vol. 50, 1989, pp. 484-486.
Dickens,
B.M.; Doob, A.N.; Warwick, O.H.; & Wingard, W.C. Report of the
Committee of Enquiry into Allegations Concerning Drs. Linda & Mark
Sobell, Addiction Research Foundation, Toronto, 1982.
Doob,
A.N. Understanding the nature of investigations into alleged fraud in
alcohol research. A reply to Walker & Roach, British Journal of
Addiction, Vol. 79, 1984, pp. 169-174.
Maltzman,
I. (unpublished a) A reply to the Sobells: You did not do what you said
you did; You did not find what you say you found.
Maltzman, I. (unpublished b) The Sobells and alchemy.
Maltzman, I. (unpublished c) Letter to the editor.
Maltzman,
I. (unpublished d) Criticisms of the Dickens Committee Enquiry into the
Sobell's alleged fraud and Doob's effort at their defense.
Maltzman,
I. A reply to Cook, “Craftsman versus professional: Analysis of the
controlled drinking controversy.” J. of Studies on Alcohol, Vol. 50,
1989, pp. 466-472.
Maltzman vs. Alcohol Research Documentation, Inc. Superior Court of California, County of Los Angeles, c681631, 1988.
Marlatt, G.A. The controlled-drinking controversy: A commentary, American Psychologist, Vol. 38, 1983, pp. 1097-1110.
Marlatt, G.A. President's Message. Bulletin of the Society of Psychologists in Addictive Behaviors, Vol. 3, 1984, p. 67.
Marlatt, G.A. Letter to Father Royce, Bulletin of the Society of Psychologists in Addictive Behaviors, Vol. 3, 1984, p. 70.
Marlatt,
G.A.; Miller, W.R.; Duckert, F.; Gotestam, G.; Heather, N.; Peele, S.;
Sanchez-Craig, M.; Sobell, L.C.; & Sobell, M.B.; Abstinence and
controlled drinking: Alternative treatment goals for alcoholism and
problem drinking? Bulletin of the Society of Psychologists in Addictive
Behaviors, Vol. 4, 1985, pp. 123- 150. Merton, R. Social theory and
social structure, Enlarged edition, The Free Press, New York, 1968.
Nathan, P.E. A prefatory comment, J. of Studies on Alcohol, Vol. 50, 1989, p.465.
Peele,
S. Denial--of reality and of freedom--in addiction research and
treatment. Bulletin of the Society of Psychologists in Addictive
Behaviors, Vol. 5, 1986, pp. 149-166.
Pendery,
M.L.; Maltzman, I.M.; and West, L.J. Controlled Drinking by alcoholics?
New findings and a reevaluation of a major affirmative study. Science,
Vol. 217, 1982, pp. 169-175.
Sobell, M.B. & Sobell, L.C.. Behavioral treatment of alcohol problems, Plenum Press, N.Y., 1978.
Sobell,
M.B. & Sobell, L.C. The aftermath of heresy. A response to Pendery
et al's (1982) critique of “Individualized behavior therapy for
alcoholics,” Behaviour Research & Therapy, Vol. 22, 1984, pp.
413-440.
Sobell,
M.B. & Sobell, L.C. More on Maltzman, Bulletin of the Society of
Psychologists in Addictive Behaviors, Vol. 3, 1984, pp. 74-76.
Sobell,
M.B. & Sobell, L.C. Moratorium on Maltzman: An appeal to reason. J.
of Studies on Alcohol, Vol. 50, 1989, pp. 473-480.
Trachtenberg,
R.L. Report of the Steering Group to the Administrator, Alcohol, Drug
Abuse, and Mental Health Administration Regarding its Attempts to
Investigate Allegations of Scientific Misconduct Concerning Drs. Mark
& Linda Sobell, Alcohol, Drug Abuse, and Mental Health
Administration, Rockville, Maryland, 1984.
Alcoholism ò Addiction, Out From Under
From: Professional Counselor magazine, 10/92